Introduction
Over the last decade, health care professionals have been exposed to the emergence of a new form of addiction—food addiction (1). Eating has always been a basic human behavior primarily devoted to the maintenance of homeostasis. Nonetheless, high-energy food products (i.e., rich in fat and/or sugar), which are everyday more available on the market, are engaging a strong reward response (2–4) and, due to those strong reinforcing properties (5), lead to addictive behaviors, like craving, similar to those caused by drugs (6). Craving, or a strong desire to consume a product, is associated with high sensitivity of the reward system (7). This hypersensitivity of the reward system has been well-documented in individuals with obesity (8–12). Notably, in response to the mere visualization of food items, neuroimaging studies report excessive activations of several brain regions strongly involved in food intake and reward processes [e.g., (9, 13)], hence fostering the excessive desire to eat (14).
Parallel to reward system hyperactivity, but less extensively investigated, a hypoactivity of the inhibition system was observed in individuals with obesity. Specifically, (f)MRI studies reported decreased gray matter volume (15) and functional activity of the frontal cortex (16), the brain region highly responsible for inhibition processes (17, 18). It has further been discovered that the repeated exposure to high-energy food will lead to a decrease of the availability of dopaminergic D2 receptors in the striatum (19). Not without consequence, this dopaminergic alteration directly impairs prefrontal activity (20, 21) and, thus, associated inhibitory control (22). Consequently, in individuals with obesity, a hypoactive inhibitory system will inevitably fail to counteract reward system hyperfunctioning (19).
Such imbalance between the two systems will behaviorally translate into an impulsive way to act (23), characterized by difficulties to override eating temptation (24) and lead to obesity. A strong association between body mass index (BMI) and impulsivity has been established in several studies (25–27). Impulsive behavior can result into poor response inhibition ability (28), which, interestingly, was found to correlate positively with overeating (29) and BMI (30, 31). Basically, those studies reported poor performances during go/no-go and stop signal tasks, with an impaired ability to inhibit from responding during no-go and stop trials. Impulsivity can also result from poor cognitive ability to override temptation. Several behavioral studies reported such insufficiently suppressed temptations in obese individuals using delay discounting, where participants have to choose between immediate or bigger although delayed reward, or craving regulation tasks, where participants are asked to use mental strategies to refrain from eating desire (32–34).
Interestingly, the presence of binge eating disorder (BED) leads to even greater impulsive traits (35, 36). This disorder, characterized by recurring short periods of uncontrolled consumption of abnormally large quantities of food, affects at least 25% of the obese population (37). Although overeating may not be considered as dangerous as drug consumption, this behavior leads to obesity with all its comorbidities (except in the case of purge, i.e., bulimia, not considered in this review since it does not lead to obesity-related devastating consequences on various aspects of health).
Recent brain stimulation studies provided promising results on the control of food craving and food intake (38). Specifically, studies showed that modulating the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (39, 40), nucleus accumbens (41), and hypothalamic area (42) substantially reduced food craving. However, inhibitory control does not exclusively rely on the functioning of those three brain regions. It rather involves a complex neural network in which key regions and their way of communication remain largely unexplored.
Considering the importance of a precise identification of the different brain areas involved in the functioning of inhibition processes for the elaboration of relevant and accurate neurocognitive therapies, we propose in this review to gather the results provided by neuroimaging studies. The main aims are to [1] provide a detailed report of the brain regions showing obesity-related impaired function during response inhibition and [2] examine whether BED leads to similar although steeper neural deterioration.
Methods
To identify studies aiming at investigating the neural basis of the inhibitory system in obese individuals, we used Google Scholar and PubMed databases using the following keywords: impulsive, impulsivity, inhibition, inhibitory system, and executive functions, each combined with the following terms: food, feeding behavior, obesity, and obese. Additional papers were also found from the reference lists of the selected papers. To comply with inclusion, studies had to [1] use a behavioral task specifically designed for the assessment of impulsivity, which [2] participants completed during functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or magnetoencephalography (MEG), and [3] entail a lean control group (except in the specific case of comparison between obese with and without binge eating disorder). Moreover, participants from the experimental group had to [4] be obese (BMI ≥ 30) and not “only” overweight (25
Results
Study Selection
The initial search identified 3,009 studies. After removal of duplicates and exclusions through title and abstract screening, 30 studies were assessed for eligibility. For design, population, and control reasons, 19 studies were excluded. After we further searched for articles on lean individuals with BED, one more article was selected. Twelve studies were hence included in this systematic review of the literature (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Prisma flow diagram.
Functional Activity
In the 12 identified studies that met the inclusion criteria (Table 1), 10 reported different patterns of activation during response inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals (n = 8) or in obese with BED in comparison with obese without BED (n = 2) and one in lean individuals with BED in comparison with lean without BED. Differences were mainly observed in the frontal (Table 2, Figure 2) and limbic regions (Table 3, Figure 3), but also in the visual and inferior parietal cortices and the Rolandic operculum (Table 4, Figure 3). One study showed an inverted U-shaped pattern of activation in the insula, claustrum, and putamen (Table 5), hence reporting disparate neural activity during inhibition depending on obesity severity (32). Hypoactivity in the frontal and visual cortices reported in obese individuals during inhibition, but also in the temporal cortex, was found to be exacerbated in obese individuals with BED (Table 6). Only Carbine et al. (45) found no difference between their obese and lean participants’ neural activity during inhibition.
Table 1. Neuroimaging studies assessing inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Table 2. Neural activity in frontal regions during inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Figure 2. Functional activity in the frontal regions during inhibition. Blue and red colors, respectively, indicate hypo- and hyperactivity, respectively, in obese in comparison with lean participants during inhibition (see Table 2 for precise coordinates). 1–3 = orbitofrontal cortex (43), 4–5 = orbitofrontal cortex (48), 6 = orbitofrontal cortex (53), 7–8 = ventromedial prefrontal cortex (43), 9 = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (48), 10 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (53), 11 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (50), 12 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (52), 13 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (43), 14 = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (43), 15–16 = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (48), 17 = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (49), 18–20 = premotor cortex (43), 21–22 = premotor cortex (48).
Table 3. Neural activity in limbic regions during inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Figure 3. Functional activity in the limbic regions during inhibition. Blue and red colors indicate hypo- and hyperactivity, respectively, in obese in comparison with lean participants during inhibition (see Table 3 for precise coordinates). 1 = anterior cingulate cortex (46), 2 = anterior cingulate cortex (53), 3–4 = posterior cingulate cortex (53), 5 = dorsal caudate nuclei (53), 6 = insula (43), 7 = insula (32), 8 = insula (32), 9 = parahippocampal gyrus (49), 10 = thalamus (49).
Table 4. Other brain regions showing different patterns of activity during inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Table 5. Regions showing an inverted U-shaped pattern of activity during inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Table 6. Regions showing different patterns of activity during inhibition in obese individuals with binge eating disorder in comparison with individuals without.
Functional Connectivity
Two from the nine identified studies assessed functional connectivity during inhibition and reported either strengthened or U-shaped patterns of connectivity in obese in comparison with lean individuals (Table 7).
Table 7. Neural connectivity during inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Discussion
The studies included in this review used go/no-go, stop signal, Stroop, and craving control tasks to investigate inhibition processes in individuals with obesity. Designed to assess response inhibition, the go/no-go (action restraint) and stop signal tasks (action cancellation) engage overlapping but also different brain activations during successful inhibition. More precisely, while the medial prefrontal cortex and the insula were found to be engaged during both tasks, the go/no-go task triggers further activation in the fronto-parietal network and the stop signal task in the cingulo-opercular network (54). The Stroop task, assessing inhibition of prepotent response tendencies, has been shown to mainly recruit the prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and posterior parietal cortices (55–57). Despite stronger cognitive engagement required during craving control tasks, the intentional regulation of food desire and response inhibition share mainly similar networks. Specifically, in comparison with passively viewed food items or allowed food craving, regulation of food craving was also found to engage a large brain network encompassing the insula, prefrontal cortex, temporal parietal junction, and the supplementary motor area (58). Here, we review to what extent those neural networks are impaired in individuals with obesity. We provide a list of brain regions together with their precise anatomical locations and discuss their potential functions during inhibition processes.
Frontal Regions
Frontal regions, including the prefrontal (PFC) and premotor cortices, have been consistently found to be involved in the inhibitory system (59–62). This role endorsed by the frontal regions in inhibitory processes also applies to the specific context of eating behavior (58). The results of this review of the literature confirm the major involvement of frontal regions during eating control in the obese population. However, over the 10 studies we identified that directly compared brain activity between lean and obese participants during an inhibition task, seven of them clearly reported an altered functioning of frontal regions, mostly characterized by hypoactivity (Figure 2).
Prefrontal Cortex
Compelling data from the studies that investigated the neural correlates of behavioral inhibition in obese in comparison with lean individuals clearly emphasize a hypoactivity of the PFC in the totality of both its medial and dorsal parts, suggesting obesity to be linked to a global impairment of this brain region, known to be a central hub for inhibitory processes.
Orbitofrontal Cortex
Located in the inferior part of the frontal lobe, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), known to be involved in encoding reward value and decision-making (63), plays a major role in the value attributed to food and subsequent eating behavior. More precisely, the lateral OFC encodes the objective nutritive value of food and integrates it within the medial OFC, which will in turn attribute the subjective value of the food item (64). Moreover, the OFC is a key node of the cognitive and, more specifically, emotion inhibition system (65). Interestingly, a decrease of the OFC metabolism has been observed in cocaine and alcohol abusers, probably on the grounds of decreased sensitivity to inhibitory (GABA) neurotransmission (20). Therefore, results from Batterink et al. (43), Hendrick et al. (48), and Tuulari et al. (53), reporting decreased activation of the OFC among obese participants during response inhibition specific to food, may suggest an alteration of the food value perception and/or emotion inhibition function(s) due to the addictive dimension of eating behavior in obese individuals.
Medial Prefrontal Cortex
The anterior part of the PFC (BA10) plays a central role in cognitive processes. BA10 is parcellated into an inferior and a superior part, respectively, named ventromedial and dorsomedial PFC. While each of these subregions endorses distinct cognitive roles, such as emotional regulation, salience attribution (66), and food valuation process (67) for the ventromedial PFC and inter alia decision-making (68–70) and uncertainty processing (71) for the dorsomedial PFC, both have been shown to play a role in inhibition processes. For instance, decreased gray matter volumes of the dorsomedial (72, 73) and lesions of the ventromedial PFC (74, 75) were found to be linked to increased levels of impulsivity. As previously observed among addicted gambler and heavy smokers (76, 77), this review of the literature revealed that obese individuals also show a hypofunctioning of both medial PFC regions during inhibition (43, 48).
Lateral Prefrontal Cortex
The lateral part of the PFC entails the dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC. Postulated to be involved in cognitive inhibition processes (78, 79), the dorsolateral PFC plays an important role in the regulation of food craving (80, 81). Essential to the downregulation of high-energy food reward, this region has been shown to be critical for dietary self-control (82). Thus, considered as a key node of eating behavior control, the dorsolateral PFC has been the subject of numerous brain stimulation studies, showing that increased activation of this region allowed an improvement of resistance to food stimuli (38). In the same vein, higher levels of activity in the dorsolateral PFC were shown to be a good predictor of diet success in obese individuals (83, 84). Surprisingly, when comparing the activity of this region between obese and lean participants, only three studies found a significant difference in activations during food-specific inhibition, characterized by a hypofunctioning BA8 (43, 50) and BA9 (53). Scharmüller et al. (52), however, reported the opposite pattern of activation in BA8 (i.e., hyperactivity in comparison with lean participants). Interestingly, beyond its implication in inhibition (85), BA8 was found to be involved in uncertainty, with increased activations being correlated to the degree of uncertainty (86, 87). This could mean that the conviction driving eating refrainment may be more unstable in obese individuals. Together, results collected from dorsolateral PFC could reflect a food-specific impairment in both response inhibition and decision-making processes.
Decreased patterns of activity were also reported in parts of both the left [BA45, (48)] and right ventrolateral PFC [BA46, (43); BA44, (48)]. The ventrolateral PFC was also shown to be involved in inhibitory processes (88) and, more particularly, to be a critical substrate of dietary self-control (89). The results from Batterink et al. (43) and Hendrick et al. (48) corroborate those from Tuulari et al. (53) observed in the dorsolateral PFC, hence extending hypofunctioning of the lateral PFC to its entire volume and supporting again the assumption of hypoactivity of key regions during eating control in obese individuals.
On the other hand, Levy and Wagner’s meta-analysis (90) revealed that the middle part of the right ventrolateral PFC (BA45) is involved in decision uncertainty. This function was found to be present only in the right ventrolateral PFC. Interestingly, Hsu et al. (49) observed that during general inhibition (i.e., non-food specific), the right BA45 was significantly more activated among obese than lean participants. Corroborating the results from Scharmüller et al. (52) in BA8, we propose that this finding may connote a decreased ability to firmly make the choice to inhibit from acting in obese individuals. However, the site of increased activation within the right BA45 was on the border of the insular region. Considering the various preprocessing steps taken to analyze fMRI data (i.e., realignment, normalization, and smoothing), the accuracy of the obtained sites of activation and related coordinates through the MRI methods bear some inaccuracies. Our assumption of greater levels of uncertainty linked to obesity should then be taken precociously, as the results from Hsu et al. (49) may also reflect insular hyperactivity.
Premotor Cortex
Situated in the superior part of the frontal cortex, Brodmann area (BA) 6 is located on the posterior part of the premotor cortex and corresponds to the supplementary motor area, also described as secondary motor cortex. Several behavioral tasks, such as the go/no-go and stop-signal task, emphasize its importance for response inhibition (85, 91, 92). In individuals with substance addiction, decreased activation of the supplementary motor area has been observed during those two inhibition tasks (93, 94). Considering the neural similarities as well as the comparable exacerbated levels of impulsivity in individuals with substance and food addiction (19, 95), results from Batterink et al. (43) and Hendrick et al. (48) are not surprising. These authors reported that, when compared with their lean counterparts, obese individuals exhibited hypoactivity of the supplementary motor area during both general and food-specific inhibition processes.
Limbic Regions
The limbic system is composed of a set of interconnected subcortical but also cortical structures. Those numerous connections form complex circuits (96), known to be highly involved in the regulation of emotion-related behavior (97).
In the present review, we report the major contributions of the limbic system to eating behavior control, but impaired among the obese population, either showing hypo- or hyperactivity in different limbic regions according to their functional role in inhibitory control (Figure 3).
Insula
As part of the gustatory cortex (98), the insula is known to play a role in smell and taste processing, as well as in fat detection (63). The insula is also a crucial region for homeostatic regulation, with its external input and expected reward integration function (99). It was more recently suggested that through the translation of objective interoceptive signals into subjective experiences such as craving, which will potentiate impulsivity, the insula may play a role in the onset and maintenance of addiction (24). For instance, experimentally silenced insula in rats and lesioned insula in humans were found to be, respectively, associated with amphetamine (100) and smoking craving disruption (101).
Consistent with the results suggesting increased insular activations during inhibition to be characteristic of inhibitory control difficulties (102) and to correlate with the tendency to eat in response to food stimuli regardless of the state of hunger in obese adolescents (103), the articles we reviewed show a hyperactivity of the insula (BA13) in obese in comparison with lean participants when inhibiting (32, 43) (see further section for the inverted U-shaped activation of the insula in relation to BMI). Only Hendrick et al. (48) found an opposite pattern of activity. However, the site of activation was on the boarder of the OFC. For the same methodological reason we mentioned before (i.e., spatial resolution accuracy), the hypoactivity found by Hendrick et al. (48) in the part of the insula bordering the OFC could potentially be attributed to the lateral OFC rather than to the insular cortex.
Cingulate Cortex Posterior Cingulate Cortex
The posterior part of the cingulate cortex is involved in processing emotionally relevant stimuli and memory-related functions (104, 105). Interestingly, its level of activation during high-calorie food anticipation was found to be associated with BMI (106). Results from Tuulari et al. (53), showing a hyperactivity of this region in obese participants in comparison with their lean counterparts, may therefore be the neural signature of excessive episodic memory-related hyperactivity during eating anticipation based on constrained downregulation and, hence, inhibition capabilities in obese individuals.
Anterior Cingulate Cortex
Besides forming an integral part of the limbic system, the anterior cingulate cortex is often considered as belonging to the frontal cortex (107) and associated inhibitory control networks (108, 109). Playing a major role in palatable food salience attribution and subsequent decision-making (63), the anterior cingulate cortex was demonstrated to be involved in the regulation of food craving and to be related to BMI. Specifically, He et al. (46) and Giuliani et al. (80) reported a negative correlation between anterior cingulate cortex activations and BMI during a food-related inhibition task. In line with those results, Tuulari et al. (53) showed that obese participants exhibited significantly lower activation in this region, therefore suggesting an impaired palatable food salience attribution with consequences on food choices in case of obesity.
Thalamus
The thalamus has been suggested to be involved in substance addiction due to its function in expectation processing (110). Expectation of the rewarding effects of drug consumption is thought to be responsible for the reinforcing dynamic of drug abuse (111). The role of the thalamus in the context of food addiction is nonetheless less elucidated. A part of the thalamus, namely the paraventricular thalamus, has been postulated to be a gateway to feeding and appetitive motivation. Specifically, it was suggested that its strategic position between brain regions responsible for homeostatic perception (i.e., hindbrain and hypothalamus), motivation, and reward processes (i.e., amygdala, ventral striatum, and cortex), allows the paraventricular thalamus to control eating behavior via bottom-up and top-down control (112). The importance of this part of the thalamus for controlling food intake was evidenced by animal research, revealing that lesions (113), pharmacological activation of its GABAA receptors (114), or its chemogenetic inhibition (115) causes increased food intake. On the contrary, activation of the paraventricular neurons was found to reduce food intake (115). Assuming that the results observed in mice would apply to human, we propose that thalamic hypoactivity in obese individuals (49) may reflect a dysfunctional inhibition of eating.
Caudate Nucleus
Together with the anterior part of the putamen, the head of the caudate nucleus represents the dorsal striatum, a region primarily responsible for reward processing (116). The mere exposure to food items is enough to produce dopamine release in this brain region (117). In the addiction literature, the caudate nucleus function is also linked to impulsivity. For instance, decreased caudate nucleus activations during the reception of a pleasant taste were associated with impulsivity and obesity (118, 119). Obesity-related lower dopamine signaling during the ingestion of food refers to the “reward-deficiency” theory, justifying the need for compensatory overeating to trigger satisfying reward responses (67). To explain the hypoactivity of the caudate nucleus during the attempt to inhibit from craving in their obese vs. lean participants, Tuulari et al. (53) referred to the reward deficiency theory. However, one may also argue that a hypoactive reward system would rather lead to improved inhibitory control. In this context, it is interesting to refer to the implication of the caudate nucleus in motor inhibition (120–122). In the study by Tuulari et al. (53), the dorsal part of the caudate nucleus was found to be hypoactive. While the ventral caudate, mainly interconnected with the limbic system, is involved in the processing of affects (123), the dorsal caudate is connected to the motor, cingular, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (124–126). Furthermore, it is thought to play a role not only in motor but also executive functions (127, 128). Therefore, an alternative interpretation of the hypoactivation of the dorsal caudate nucleus during attempting to inhibit the urge to eat in obese individuals could originate from impaired motor and/or cognitive inhibitory functions.
Parahippocampal Gyrus
As part of the reward system, the parahippocampal gyrus is involved in hedonic feeding and incentive motivation processes (129). Its activity was shown to be responsive to the perception of food items (130) and to correlate positively with obesity (9) and weight gain (131). The role of the parahippocampal gyrus during reward processing seems to depend on its function in emotional memory (129, 132). In Chen et al. (133), trait-based food craving was associated with spontaneous neuronal activity in the parahippocampal gyrus. This suggests reinforced food-related hedonic memories in obese individuals. However, the hypersensitivity of the food reward system is rather counterintuitive to explain the results from Hsu et al. (49), reporting a hypoactivity of the right parahippocampal gyrus (BA36) during inhibition among obese in comparison with lean individuals.
Besides its role in reward and memory functions, the parahippocampal gyrus also seems to underpin inhibition. For instance, using the go/no-go task in healthy adults, Nakata et al. (134) revealed the implication of this brain region in inhibition processes. Its activity was further shown to positively correlate with inhibition success (135). During no-go trials, normally developing children activated a neural network also comprising the parahippocampal gyrus, whereas children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, which is associated with impaired inhibition capacities (136), failed to activate this region during inhibition attempts (137). In the literature specific to addiction, Sheinkopf et al. (138) revealed that activations of the parahippocampal gyrus during inhibition were significantly attenuated in children with prenatal exposure to cocaine. Despite the lack of information specifically concerning the role of this region in inhibition processes in individuals with food addiction, we propose that the hypoactivity observed by Hsu et al. (49) during no-go trials is likely to reflect an impairment of the inhibitory function hosted by the parahippocampal gyrus in the obese population.
Additional Regions
Visual Cortex
Located in the posterior part of the occipital cortex, the cuneus belongs to the visual cortex. Besides its essential role in the processing of visual information (139), the cuneus has been shown to be involved in addiction. Both structural and functional impairments of the cuneus have been suggested to be associated with addiction disorders. For instance, decreased cuneus volume was found to negatively correlate with years of drug use in cocaine addicts (140) and to predict relapse in alcoholics (141). In the same vein, using a color-word drug Stroop task, Goldstein et al. (142) found hypoactivation of the cuneus during inhibition in cocaine addicts. Although the structural and functional properties of the cuneus in individuals with food addiction remain to be elucidated, we propose that the hypoactivity reported by Hendrick et al. (48) in obese in comparison with lean individuals during inhibition (Figure 4) may stem from altered cuneus volume and/or function.
Figure 4. Functional activity in other regions during inhibition. Blue color indicates hypoactivity in obese in comparison with lean individuals during inhibition (see Table 4 for precise coordinates). 1–5 = visual cortex (48), 6–7 = rolandic operculum (49), 8–9 = inferior parietal cortex (48).
Rolandic Operculum
Studies investigating the neural responses to both food anticipation and delivery revealed a hyper-responsivity of the Rolandic operculum in obese adults (143) and adolescents (144), hence suggesting this brain region to be part of the food reward system. Decreased activations of the Rolandic operculum should therefore concur with decreased reward responses, hence facilitating inhibition processes. In that sense, its hypoactivity reported by Hsu et al. (49) in obese individuals during inhibition may seem counterintuitive. This result rather suggests that the Rolandic operculum may actively play a role not only in reward but also in inhibition processes related to food behavior. In drug addicts, studies reported decreased gray matter volume in this brain region (145, 146). Likewise, using single-photon emission computed tomography, Willeumier et al. (147) scanned a cohort of suicide, also known to present excessive levels of impulsivity (148). The authors revealed decreased cerebral blood flow in the region of the Rolandic operculum. Considering those results, we assume that the hypoactivity of the Rolandic operculum during inhibition among obese individuals (49) (Figure 4) may stem from hypoperfusion and/or atrophy, resulting from high impulsivity levels and, in turn, leading to inadequate response inhibition.
Inferior Parietal Cortex
The inferior parietal cortex has been shown to be consistently involved in response inhibition processes, measured either with go/no-go (149), stop signal reaction time (150), or other adapted tasks (151, 152). Interestingly, in populations showing high levels of impulsivity, such as individuals with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and alcoholics, voxel-based morphometry analyses showed decreased gray matter volume of the inferior parietal cortex (153, 154). The deleterious effects of impulsivity were also found to not only impact inferior parietal cortex structure but also its functionality. Notably, Schilling et al. (155) reported decreased gray matter volumes and Horn et al. (102) showed decreased neural activity during no-go trials. In the specific context of obesity, Stoeckel et al. (156) revealed that the inferior parietal cortex was less activated during difficult than easy delay-discounting trials. To that extent, we propose that the hypoactivity of the inferior parietal cortex (BA40) reported by Hendrick et al. (48) in obese individuals during stop signals (Figure 4) reflects the impairment of inhibitory capacities and, hence, the amplification of impulsivity, probably as a consequence of structural and/or functional alterations.
Inverted U-Shaped Reward Activations
In their study, Dietrich et al. (32) found an inverted U-shaped pattern of activation in the insula, claustrum, and putamen during inhibition (Figure 5), with the highest activation values observed among class I obese participants (BMI = 30). With its role merging the detection of food characteristics and interoception, the insula plays a major role in reward processing. Increased insular activations in class I obese participants support the assumption of a hypersensitivity of the reward system in addicts (13, 95, 144). A similar pattern of activation was obtained from the putamen, which together with the caput caudatum constitutes the ventral striatum, a key region of the brain’s reward system (157). The putamen entails both the delivery and the detection of reward (158) and is part of the corticolimbic pathway controlling food reward processes (67). Moreover, supporting habit formation (159), the putamen is involved in the mediation of habitual eating behavior (67). The creation of a habit, originating from previous goal-directed behavior, is accelerated through dopamine release (160). Obesity, and thus habitual overeating, was found to be positively associated with dopamine D2-like receptors in the putamen. This suggests that habitual excessive eating behavior observed in obese individuals may partly stem from the putamen’s sensitivity to enhanced dopamine release. Another explanation for this increased activation of the putamen in class I obese individuals during inhibition is that highly caloric foods act as strong reward reinforcers (19) and may thus hamper automatic eating behavior control, hence leading to overeating (161). This loss of control resulting from exposure to salient stimulus refers to an attentional bias. In individuals with substance addiction, attentional bias was found to positively correlate with craving and putamen activations (162).
Figure 5. Inverted U-shaped pattern of functional activity during inhibition. Green color indicates an increased neural activity in class-I obese (BMI = 30) in comparison with lean participants, and an attenuated activity in obesity classes II and III (BMI = 35–40) reaching similar patterns of activation as in lean participants (see Table 5 for precise coordinates). 1 = insula (32), 2 = claustrum (32), 3 = Putamen (32).
Between the insula and the putamen lies the claustrum, a very small brain region [i.e., roughly 0.25% of the cerebral cortex (163)], but nonetheless highly connected (164). It was found to be also hyperactive among class I obese. Due to its bilateral connections with all areas of the cortex, the claustrum has been identified as a key node for multisensory integration and then responsible for the encoding of stimuli salience (165). To reduce the affluence of cortical information not selected for attention, the claustrum operates via a selective activation of inhibitory neurons in layer IV (166). The hyperactivity of this region among obese individuals during inhibition could be the sign of an exacerbated inhibitory activity aiming at focusing attention through compensating the excessive intensity of perception-related cortical processing. This hypothesis would reflect an excessive sensorial input and/or difficulty in deciding for the salience qualities of a given food-related input in class I obese individuals.
Altogether, those results support the notion of a hypersensitive reward system in obese individuals, but only partially. While activation in these areas linearly increases up to a BMI of 30, a BMI from 30 to 40 is associated with the opposite dynamic, suggesting a decreased hypersensitivity of the reward system in class II obese individuals. Parallel to reward hypersensitivity, the reward hyposensitivity hypothesis can also be found in the addiction literature [i.e., reward deficiency theory (95, 144)]. While the former applies during eating anticipation, the later occurs during actual reception. According to the reward deficiency theory, obese individuals must consume more food to reach satisfying reward activations and subsequent pleasure perception (67). Neurobiological evidence for this assumption relies on decreased dopamine D2 receptor availability in obese individuals’ brain (167). Results from Dietrich et al. (32) suggest that this alteration of the dopaminergic system may be BMI dependent and within the different obese classes. The inverted U-shaped pattern of activation these authors found in the insula, putamen, and claustrum could signify that the dynamics of the impaired dopaminergic system may be reversed with increasing obesity severity and that the reward deficiency theory also applies to food anticipation in severely obese individuals.
Functional Connectivity
Functional connectivity studies showed a stronger connectivity of frontal regions (dorsolateral PFC and pre-supplementary motor area) with the putamen, cingulate cortex, supplementary motor area, precuneus, and inferior parietal cortex (53) (Figure 6), suggesting a shift in neural demand away from executive to hedonic functions during inhibition of food intake. This strengthened functional connectivity between inhibition and reward-related nodes was furthermore found to be a two-way relationship, notably between the inferior parietal cortex (precuneus), the basal ganglia (putamen), and frontal regions (dorsolateral PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventrolateral PFC, and supplementary motor area) (32, 53). As the putamen is involved in the attribution of food reward value and in the mediation of habitual eating behavior (see previous section), and the precuneus plays a role in a large number of affective and cognitive functions (168), the strengthened functional connectivity between those reward regions with the frontal part of the brain may also reflect increased neural demand for the successful control of food intake in the obese population. The hyperactive interplay between frontal and limbic regions during inhibition in obese individuals may be the adaptive signature of increased neural activity aiming at compensating the pathologically induced decreased functioning of those regions.
Figure 6. Functional connectivity during inhibition. Red and green connections indicate hyperconnectivity and U-shaped connectivity, respectively, in obese in comparison with lean participants during inhibition (see Table 7 for precise coordinates). Regions n°1, 5, 10, 14, and 21 are seed regions. 1 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 2 = putamen, 3 = cingulate cortex, 4 = supplementary motor area, 5 = pre-supplementary motor area, 6 = precuneus, 7 = cingulate cortex, 8–9 = parietal cortex, 10 = precuneus, 11 = supplementary motor area, 12 = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, 13 = sensory cortex (53), 14 = putamen, 15–19 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 20 = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, 21 = amygdala, 22 = pallidum, 23 = putamen, 24–25 = visual cortex (32).
Conversely, a U-shaped pattern of functional connectivity was reported between the amygdala, involved in motivational salience encoding (169), and regions involved in reward perception and signaling (32) (Figure 6). Whereas, the functional activity in lean and severely obese individuals was found to be similar, decreased values were reported in class I obese individuals between the amygdala, visual cortex, and basal ganglia (pallidum and putamen). Dietrich et al. (32) assumed that BMI may affect craving regulation through a U-shaped modulation of the interplay between salience encoding and pleasantness computation. This would mean that whereas mildly obese individuals show a decreased functional connectivity of the regions involved in craving regulation, morbidly obese individuals’ neural activity is similar to the pattern obtained from normal weighted controls, despite an exacerbated behavioral loss of control regarding food craving regulation (probably even greater than among mildly obese individuals). This assumption seems counterintuitive to us. We therefore assume that the U-shaped pattern of functional connectivity found in this brain network may rely on other explanations, however still unknown and deserving further examination.
Binge Eating Disorder
Part of the obese population is confronted with an aggravated form of excessive eating behavior, consisting in consuming a large amount of food within a short period of time, paired with a sensation of loss of control. This pathological eating behavior, named BED, is referenced as a stand-alone illness in the DSM-5 (170). As binge period may be followed by purge (e.g., vomiting), not all individuals suffering from BED are obese. This is however true in 40% of the cases (171). Importantly, in comparison with obese individuals without BED, those presenting the pathology were found to have higher psychiatric comorbidities and diabetes rates, as well as more physical symptom and health dissatisfaction (172–175). Considering those alarming observations, a deeper understanding of the disinhibition processes underlying BED, and specifically among the obese population, is critical. As behavioral impulsivity was found to be even more pronounced among obese with BED (176, 177), it can be expected that the neural networks responsible for inhibition, already found to be downregulated in the obese population (see previous sections), are presenting further weakening in the presence of BED. Results from Balodis et al. (44) and Hege et al. (47) support this assumption. Using food-specific Stroop color-word interference and go/no-go tasks, these authors emphasize the exacerbated decrease of frontal regions’ activity. Specifically, obese individuals with BED showed less neural activation within the ventrolateral (BA46) and dorsolateral part (BA9) of the prefrontal cortex. The same impairment was also observed in the visual cortex, more precisely in the cuneus (BA19). Besides this exacerbation of the frontal and visual regions’ hypoactivity, the study from Balodis et al. (44) further revealed hypofunctioning of the middle (BA21) and inferior parts (BA37) of the temporal lobe. Belonging to the cognitive system (178, 179), the temporal lobe is also involved in inhibition processes (180, 181). Higher impulsivity levels have been found to be associated with reduced gray matter volume (182) and regional cerebral blood flow (183) in the temporal lobe. Moreover, animal studies showed that structural alterations of the temporal cortex led to hyperphagia and obesity (184). This relation between temporal cortex integrity and eating behavior was also observed among humans. Gray matter volume was notably found to be negatively correlated with BMI (185, 186) and, accordingly, systematically reduced in obese individuals (187–189). A 24-year longitudinal study showed that individuals who were starting to increase their BMI during their middle age present decreased gray matter volume in the temporal lobe at a later age (190). This suggests that this brain alteration is a consequence from obesity, probably because of related insulin resistance (15). However, women with an atrophy of the temporal lobe were found to have a higher BMI (190), here suggesting the reversed cause–effect relationship between obesity and the temporal lobe. In essence, results from Balodis et al. (44) and Hege et al. (47) support the assumption of similar, although exacerbated, as well as further inhibition network impairments in obese with binge eating disorder (Figure 7).
Figure 7. Functional activity in individuals with binge eating disorder during inhibition. Blue color indicates hypoactivity in obese individuals with binge eating disorder in comparison with obese individuals without binge eating disorder during inhibition. Yellow and orange colors indicate hypo- and hyperactivity, respectively, in lean with binge eating disorder in comparison with lean without binge eating disorder (see Table 6 for precise coordinates). 1–2 = ventromedial prefrontal cortex (51), 3 = dorsomedial prefrontal prefrontal cortex (51), 4 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (47), 5 = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (51), 6 = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (44), 7–8 = visual cortex (44), 9 = visual cortex (51), 10 = primary motor cortex (51), 11–12 = premotor cortex (51), 13 = primary sensory motor cortex (51), 14–19 = cerebellum (51), 20–23 = precuneus (51), 24–25 = putamen (51), 26 = temporal gyrus (44).
Binge eating episodes can also occur in normal-weight individuals (191). They are thought to occur more frequently and compulsively in the course of time (192), hence increasing the risk to develop obesity. To provide a better understanding of the tendency to binge eating, Oliva et al. (51) investigated neural activity of normal-weight adults while performing the go/no-go and stop signal inhibition tasks. Inconsistent with the results from Hege et al. (47) who used the classical version of the go/no-go task in obese with BED, Oliva et al. (51) reported increased activity of the dlPFC during the food-specific version of the stop signal task. Those results are however consistent with the findings from Scharmüller et al. (52) who also used a food-specific inhibition task. This suggests that the opposite patterns of activity observed in the dlPFC in lean individuals with BED in comparison with obese individuals with BED may not only steam from the presence of obesity but also from the nature of the inhibition task. This may also explain the increased activity observed in the visual cortex in lean individuals with BED during inhibition, whereas those with BED and obesity were shown to present hypoactivity (44). Besides those incongruent results with obese BED, lean BED further showed hypoactivity of the primary sensorimotor cortex, precuneus, putamen, and cerebellum, all being involved in inhibition processes (59, 193–195). To what extent an impairment of those regions during inhibition may represent early warning signs of obesity development deserves further longitudinal investigation.
Limitations
This review article entails studies that used general as well as food-specific paradigms to investigate inhibition processes, despite coming from different methodological paradigms (i.e., experimental tasks). Thus, this precludes obtaining information on the general or specific aspect of inhibition impairment in individuals with obesity and BED. Moreover, only 12 studies could be included, hence not allowing detailed and firm conclusions to be drawn. That is why we aimed at additionally adding EEG studies. However, all studies we found, although they showed impaired inhibition response, did not report on precise brain regions. Thus, we did not include EEG studies in this review.
Conclusion
In this review, we gathered further evidence of shared neural impairment underlying drug and food addiction, mainly targeting the frontal and limbic regions. Specifically, obesity is linked to alterations of the dopaminergic system during response inhibition, translating into hypoactivity of the frontal regions and either hypo- or hyperactivity of the limbic regions according to their role in behavior control. We further conclude that the presence of BED may lead to similar although greater impairment of the inhibition system. We however suggest that a deeper and more grounded understanding of the inhibition system impairment related to obesity should constitute the subject matter of future studies. Investigating whether alterations of the dopaminergic function and its related inhibition processes in obese individuals are rather general or specific to eating behavior, and also whether it varies within different classes of obesity, could provide valuable insights for the comprehension of inhibition impairment in the obese population.
In summary, inhibition processes are found to rely on a complex neural network involving the prefrontal but also the frontal and subcortical regions. Whether stimulation of the identified key areas, such as the striatum, orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal, cingulate, or insular cortex, may lead to substantial improved eating control deserves future non-invasive (transcranial direct current stimulation, repetitive transcranial magnet stimulation) as well as invasive brain stimulation studies (deep brain stimulation).
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author Contributions
ES wrote the manuscript. BP revised the manuscript. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by the IFB AdiposityDiseases and the nutriCARD initiative (http://www.nutricard.de), Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF), Germany, FKZ: 01E01001 (http://www.bmbf.de), and the German Research Foundation (DFG) (http://www.dfg.de), within the framework of the CRC 1052 Obesity Mechanisms, project TP 6, to BP, and the CRC 874 Integration and Representation of Sensory Processes, project TP10, to BP.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
4. Weltens N, Zhao D, Van Oudenhove L. Where is the comfort in comfort foods? Mechanisms linking fat signaling, reward, and emotion. Neurogastroenterol Motil. (2014) 26:303–15. doi: 10.1111/nmo.12309
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
6. DiFeliceantonio AG, Small DM. Dopamine and diet-induced obesity. Nat Neurosci. (2019) 22:1–2. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0304-0
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
7. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F, Baler R. Addiction: decreased reward sensitivity and increased expectation sensitivity conspire to overwhelm the brain’s control circuit. BioEssays News Rev Mol Cell Dev Biol. (2010) 32:748–55. doi: 10.1002/bies.201000042
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
9. Brooks SJ, Cedernaes J, Schiöth HB. Increased prefrontal and parahippocampal activation with reduced dorsolateral prefrontal and insular cortex activation to food images in obesity: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e60393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060393
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
11. Gearhardt AN, Yokum S, Orr PT, Stice E, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Neural correlates of food addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2011) 68:808–16. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.32
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
12. Stice E, Figlewicz DP, Gosnell BA, Levine AS, Pratt WE. The contribution of brain reward circuits to the obesity epidemic. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:2047–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
13. Stoeckel LE, Weller RE, Cook EW, Twieg DB, Knowlton RC, Cox JE. Widespread reward-system activation in obese women in response to pictures of high-calorie foods. NeuroImage. (2008) 41:636–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.031
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
14. Mela DJ. Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite. (2006) 47:10–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.02.006
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
15. Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, Convit A. Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity. (2011) 19:1382–7. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
16. Lavagnino L, Arnone D, Cao B, Soares JC, Selvaraj S. Inhibitory control in obesity and binge eating disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 68:714–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.041
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
17. Dillon DG, Pizzagalli DA. Inhibition of action, thought, and emotion: a selective neurobiological review. Appl Prev Psychol. (2007) 12:99–114. doi: 10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
18. Ridderinkhof KR, Van Den Wildenberg WP, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. (2004) 56:129–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
19. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3191–200. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
21. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Begleiter H, Porjesz B, Fowler JS, Telang F, et al. High levels of dopamine D2 receptors in unaffected members of alcoholic families: possible protective factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2006) 63:999–1008. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.9.999
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
22. Fineberg NA, Potenza MN, Chamberlain SR, Berlin HA, Menzies L, Bechara A, et al. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. (2010) 35:591–604. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.185
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
23. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. (1999) 146:373–90. doi: 10.1007/PL00005483
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
24. Chen R, Li DP, Turel O, Sørensen TA, Bechara A, Li Y, et al. Decision making deficits in relation to food cues influence obesity: a triadic neural model of problematic eating. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9:264. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00264
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
25. Murphy CM, Stojek MK, MacKillop J. Interrelationships among impulsive personality traits, food addiction, and body mass index. Appetite. (2014) 73:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
28. Logan GD, Schachar RJ, Tannock R. Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol Sci. (1997) 8:60–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00545.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
29. Guerrieri R, Nederkoorn C, Stankiewicz K, Alberts H, Geschwind N, Martijn C, et al. The influence of trait and induced state impulsivity on food intake in normal-weight healthy women. Appetite. (2007) 49:66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.11.008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
30. Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, et al. The association of childhood obesity to neuroelectric indices of inhibition. Psychophysiology. (2012) 49:1361–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01459.x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
31. Nederkoorn C, Smulders FTY, Havermans RC, Roefs A, Jansen A. Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite. (2006) 47:253–6. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.008
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
32. Dietrich A, Hollmann M, Mathar D, Villringer A, Horstmann A. Brain regulation of food craving: relationships with weight status and eating behavior. Int J Obes. (2016) 40:982–9. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2016.28
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
33. Mole TB, Irvine MA, Worbe Y, Collins P, Mitchell SP, Bolton S, et al. Impulsivity in disorders of food and drug misuse. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:771–82. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001834
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
35. Schag K, Schönleber J, Teufel M, Zipfel S, Giel KE. Food-related impulsivity in obesity and binge eating disorder–a systematic review. Obes Rev. (2013) 14:477–95. doi: 10.1111/obr.12017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
36. Giel K, Teufel M, Junne F, Zipfel S, Schag K. Food-related impulsivity in obesity and binge eating disorder—a systematic update of the evidence. Nutrients. (2017) 9:1170. doi: 10.3390/nu9111170
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
39. Goldman RL, Borckardt JJ, Frohman HA, O’Neil PM, Madan A, Campbell LK, et al. Prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) temporarily reduces food cravings and increases the self-reported ability to resist food in adults with frequent food craving. Appetite. (2011) 56:741–6. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.013
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
40. Kekic M, McClelland J, Campbell I, Nestler S, Rubia K, David AS, et al. The effects of prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on food craving and temporal discounting in women with frequent food cravings. Appetite. (2014) 78:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.010
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
41. Harat M, Rudaś M, Zieliński P, Birska J, Sokal P. Nucleus accumbens stimulation in pathological obesity. Neurol Neurochir Pol. (2016) 50:207–10. doi: 10.1016/j.pjnns.2016.01.014
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
42. Whiting DM, Tomycz ND, Bailes J, de Jonge L, Lecoultre V, Wilent B, et al. Lateral hypothalamic area deep brain stimulation for refractory obesity: a pilot study with preliminary data on safety, body weight, and energy metabolism. J Neurosurg. (2013) 119:56–63. doi: 10.3171/2013.2.JNS12903
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
43. Batterink L, Yokum S, Stice E. Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. (2010) 52:1696–703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
44. Balodis IM, Molina ND, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, White MA, Sinha R, et al. Divergent neural substrates of inhibitory control in binge eating disorder relative to other manifestations of obesity. Obesity. (2013) 21:367–77. doi: 10.1002/oby.20068
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
45. Carbine KA, Duraccio KM, Kirwan CB, Muncy NM, LeCheminant JD, Larson MJ. A direct comparison between ERP and fMRI measurements of food-related inhibitory control: implications for BMI status and dietary intake. NeuroImage. (2018) 166:335–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
46. He Q, Xiao L, Xue G, Wong S, Ames SL, Schembre SM, et al. Poor ability to resist tempting calorie rich food is linked to altered balance between neural systems involved in urge and self-control. Nutr J. (2014) 13:92. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-13-92
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
47. Hege MA, Stingl KT, Kullmann S, Schag K, Giel KE, Zipfel S, et al. Attentional impulsivity in binge eating disorder modulates response inhibition performance and frontal brain networks. Int J Obes. (2015) 39:353–60. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.99
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
48. Hendrick OM, Luo X, Zhang S, Li C-SR. Saliency processing and obesity: a preliminary imaging study of the stop signal task. Obesity. (2012) 20:1796–802. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.180
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
49. Hsu J-S, Wang P-W, Ko C-H, Hsieh T-J, Chen C-Y, Yen J-Y. Altered brain correlates of response inhibition and error processing in females with obesity and sweet food addiction: a functional magnetic imaging study. Obes Res Clin Pract. (2017) 11:677–86. doi: 10.1016/j.orcp.2017.04.011
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
50. Janssen LK, Duif I, van Loon I, Wegman J, de Vries JHM, Cools R, et al. Loss of lateral prefrontal cortex control in food-directed attention and goal-directed food choice in obesity. NeuroImage. (2017) 146:148–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.015
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
51. Oliva R, Morys F, Horstmann A, Castiello U, Begliomini C. The impulsive brain: neural underpinnings of binge eating behavior in normal-weight adults. Appetite. (2019) 136:33–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.043
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
52. Scharmüller W, Übel S, Ebner F, Schienle A. Appetite regulation during food cue exposure: a comparison of normal-weight and obese women. Neurosci Lett. (2012) 518:106–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.04.063
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
53. Tuulari JJ, Karlsson HK, Hirvonen J, Salminen P, Nuutila P, Nummenmaa L. Neural circuits for cognitive appetite control in healthy and obese individuals: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0116640. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116640
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
54. Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage. (2011) 56:1655–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.070
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
55. Banich MT, Milham MP, Atchley R, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Wszalek T, et al. fMRI studies of stroop tasks reveal unique roles of anterior and posterior brain systems in attentional selection. J Cogn Neurosci. (2000) 12:988–1000. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137521
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
56. Barch DM, Braver TS, Akbudak E, Conturo T, Ollinger J, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. Cereb Cortex N Y N. (2001) 11:837–48. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.837
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
57. MacDonald AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. (2000) 288:1835–8. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
58. Han JE, Boachie N, Garcia-Garcia I, Michaud A, Dagher A. Neural correlates of dietary self-control in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Physiol Behav. (2018) 192:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.037
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
59. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1285–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
60. Buchsbaum BR, Greer S, Chang W-L, Berman KF. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting task and component processes. Hum Brain Mapp. (2005) 25:35–45. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20128
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
62. Duque J, Labruna L, Verset S, Olivier E, Ivry RB. Dissociating the role of prefrontal and premotor cortices in controlling inhibitory mechanisms during motor preparation. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:806–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4299-12.2012
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
63. Clarke RE, Verdejo-Garcia A, Andrews ZB. The role of corticostriatal–hypothalamic neural circuits in feeding behaviour: implications for obesity. J Neurochem. (2018) 147:715–29. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14455
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
64. Suzuki S, Cross L, O’Doherty JP. Elucidating the underlying components of food valuation in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. (2017) 20:1780–6. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0008-x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
65. Hooker CI, Knight RT. The role of lateral orbitofrontal cortex in the inhibitory control of emotion. Orbitofrontal Cortex. (2006) 307:307–24. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198565741.003.0012
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
67. Hollmann M, Pleger B, Villringer A, Horstmann A. Brain imaging in the context of food perception and eating. Curr Opin Lipidol. (2013) 24:18–24. doi: 10.1097/MOL.0b013e32835b61a4
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
68. Rushworth MFS, Kennerley SW, Walton ME. Cognitive neuroscience: resolving conflict in and over the medial frontal cortex. Curr Biol. (2005) 15:R54–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.054
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
69. Venkatraman V, Payne JW, Bettman JR, Luce MF, Huettel SA. Separate neural mechanisms underlie choices and strategic preferences in risky decision making. Neuron. (2009) 62:593–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
70. Venkatraman V, Rosati AG, Taren AA, Huettel SA. Resolving response, decision, and strategic control: evidence for a functional topography in dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:13158–64. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2708-09.2009
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
71. Bzdok D, Langner R, Schilbach L, Engemann DA, Laird AR, Fox PT, et al. Segregation of the human medial prefrontal cortex in social cognition. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:232. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00232
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
73. Du X, Qi X, Yang Y, Du G, Gao P, Zhang Y, et al. Corrigendum: altered structural correlates of impulsivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 13:124. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00124
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
74. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. (2000) 123:2189–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
76. de Ruiter MB, Oosterlaan J, Veltman DJ, van den Brink W, Goudriaan AE. Similar hyporesponsiveness of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in problem gamblers and heavy smokers during an inhibitory control task. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2012) 121:81–9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.010
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
77. Potenza MN, Leung H-C, Blumberg HP, Peterson BS, Fulbright RK, Lacadie CM, et al. An fMRI stroop task study of ventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gamblers. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:1990–4. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1990
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
78. Knoch D, Brugger P, Regard M. Suppressing versus releasing a habit: frequency-dependent effects of prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex. (2005) 15:885–7. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh196
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
79. Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Baeken C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and stroop performance: tackling the lateralization. Psychon Bull Rev. (2009) 16:609–12. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.3.609
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
80. Giuliani NR, Mann T, Tomiyama AJ, Berkman ET. Neural systems underlying the reappraisal of personally craved foods. J Cogn Neurosci. (2014) 26:1390–402. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00563
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
81. Hollmann M, Hellrung L, Pleger B, Schlögl H, Kabisch S, Stumvoll M, et al. Neural correlates of the volitional regulation of the desire for food. Int J Obes. (2012) 36:648–55. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.125
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
83. Weygandt M, Mai K, Dommes E, Leupelt V, Hackmack K, Kahnt T, et al. The role of neural impulse control mechanisms for dietary success in obesity. Neuroimage. (2013) 83:669–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.028
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
84. Weygandt M, Mai K, Dommes E, Ritter K, Leupelt V, Spranger J, et al. Impulse control in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex counteracts post-diet weight regain in obesity. NeuroImage. (2015) 109:318–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.073
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
85. Li CR, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R. Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:186–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3741-05.2006
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
86. Volz KG, Schubotz RI, Cramon DY. Variants of uncertainty in decision-making and their neural correlates. Brain Res Bull. (2005) 67:403–12. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.011
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
87. Dekleva BM, Ramkumar P, Wanda PA, Kording KP, Miller LE. Uncertainty leads to persistent effects on reach representations in dorsal premotor cortex. eLife. (2016) 5:e14316. doi: 10.7554/eLife.14316
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
88. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
89. Maier SU, Hare TA. Higher heart-rate variability is associated with ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity and increased resistance to temptation in dietary self-control challenges. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:446–55. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2815-16.2017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
90. Levy BJ, Wagner AD. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2011) 1224:40–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05958.x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
91. Limongi R, Pérez FJ. Successful and unsuccessful response inhibitions differentially affect the effective connectivity between insular, presupplementary-motor, and striatal areas. Behav Neurosci. (2017) 131:20–32. doi: 10.1037/bne0000175
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
92. Li C-SR, Yan P, Chao HH-A, Sinha R, Paliwal P, Constable RT, et al. Error-specific medial cortical and subcortical activity during the stop signal task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience. (2008) 155:1142–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.062
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
93. Feil J, Sheppard D, Fitzgerald PB, Yücel M, Lubman DI, Bradshaw JL. Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2010) 35:248–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
94. Weafer J, Dzemidzic M, Eiler W II, Oberlin BG, Wang Y, Kareken DA. Associations between regional brain physiology and trait impulsivity, motor inhibition, and impaired control over drinking. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2015) 233:81–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.04.010
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
95. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Baler R. Food and drug reward: overlapping circuits in human obesity and addiction. In: Carter CS, Dalley JW, editors. Brain Imaging in Behavioral Neuroscience. Berlin: Springer (2012). p. 1–24. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_169
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
96. Hensler JG. Serotonergic modulation of the limbic system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2006) 30:203–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.007
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
98. Veldhuizen MG, Albrecht J, Zelano C, Boesveldt S, Breslin P, Lundström JN. Identification of human gustatory cortex by activation likelihood estimation. Hum Brain Mapp. (2011) 32:2256–66. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21188
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
100. Contreras M, Ceric F, Torrealba F. Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science. (2007) 318:655–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1145590
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
102. Horn NR, Dolan M, Elliott R, Deakin JFW, Woodruff PWR. Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. (2003) 41:1959–66. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00077-0
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
103. Mata F, Verdejo-Roman J, Soriano-Mas C, Verdejo-Garcia A. Insula tuning towards external eating versus interoceptive input in adolescents with overweight and obesity. Appetite. (2015) 93:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.024
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
104. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum Brain Mapp. (2003) 18:30–41. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10075
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
105. Fink GR, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, Bruckbauer T, Kessler J, Heiss W-D. Cerebral representation of one’s own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J Neurosci. (1996) 16:4275–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04275.1996
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
106. Rothemund Y, Preuschhof C, Bohner G, Bauknecht H-C, Klingebiel R, Flor H, et al. Differential activation of the dorsal striatum by high-calorie visual food stimuli in obese individuals. NeuroImage. (2007) 37:410–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
107. Ballmaier M, Toga AW, Blanton RE, Sowell ER, Lavretsky H, Peterson J, et al. Anterior cingulate, gyrus rectus, and orbitofrontal abnormalities in elderly depressed patients: an MRI-based parcellation of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. (2004) 161:99–108. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.1.99
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
108. Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb Cortex. (2001) 11:825–36. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.825
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
109. Menon V, Adleman NE, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum Brain Mapp. (2001) 12:131–43. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200103)12:3<131::AID-HBM1010>3.0.CO;2-C
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
110. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Zhu W, Maynard L, et al. Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and the reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers. J Neurosci. (2003) 23:11461–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-36-11461.2003
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
111. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. (1993) 18:247–91. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
112. Millan EZ, Ong Z, McNally GP. Paraventricular thalamus: gateway to feeding, appetitive motivation, and drug addiction. In: Calvey T, Daniels WMU, editors. Progress in Brain Research. London: Elsevier (2017). p. 113–37.
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
113. Bhatnagar S, Dallman MF. The paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus alters rhythms in core temperature and energy balance in a state-dependent manner. Brain Res. (1999) 851:66–75. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)02108-3
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
114. Stratford TR, Wirtshafter D. Injections of muscimol into the paraventricular thalamic nucleus, but not mediodorsal thalamic nuclei, induce feeding in rats. Brain Res. (2013) 1490:128–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.043
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
117. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, Franceschi D, et al. “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect. Synapse. (2002) 44:175–80. doi: 10.1002/syn.10075
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
118. Green E, Jacobson A, Haase L, Murphy C. Reduced nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus activation to a pleasant taste is associated with obesity in older adults. Brain Res. (2011) 1386:109–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.071
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
119. Babbs RK, Sun X, Felsted J, Chouinard-Decorte F, Veldhuizen MG, Small DM. Decreased caudate response to milkshake is associated with higher body mass index and greater impulsivity. Physiol Behav. (2013) 121:103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.025
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
120. Buchwald NA, Wyers EJ, Lauprecht CW, Heuser G. The “caudate-spindle” IV. A behavioral index of caudate-induced inhibition. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1961) 13:531–7. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(61)90167-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
122. Aron AR, Schlaghecken F, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Eimer M, Barker R, et al. Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington’s disease. Brain. (2003) 126:713–23. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg067
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
123. Martikainen IK, Nuechterlein EB, Peciña M, Love TM, Cummiford CM, Green CR, et al. Chronic back pain is associated with alterations in dopamine neurotransmission in the ventral striatum. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:9957–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-14.2015
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
124. Postuma RB, Dagher A. Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a meta-analysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. Cereb Cortex. (2006) 16:1508–21. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj088
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
125. Leh SE, Ptito A, Chakravarty MM, Strafella AP. Fronto-striatal connections in the human brain: a probabilistic diffusion tractography study. Neurosci Lett. (2007) 419:113–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.049
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
126. Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Shehzad Z, et al. Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. (2008) 18:2735–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn041
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
127. Choi EY, Yeo BTT, Buckner RL. The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 108:2242–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.00270.2012
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
128. Jung WH, Jang JH, Park JW, Kim E, Goo E-H, Im O-S, et al. Unravelling the intrinsic functional organization of the human striatum: a parcellation and connectivity study based on resting-state fMRI. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e106768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106768
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
129. Bragulat V, Dzemidzic M, Bruno C, Cox CA, Talavage T, Considine RV, et al. Food-related odor probes of brain reward circuits during hunger: a pilot fMRI study. Obesity. (2010) 18:1566–71. doi: 10.1038/oby.2010.57
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
130. St-Onge M-P, Sy M, Heymsfield SB, Hirsch J. Human cortical specialization for food: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. J Nutr. (2005) 135:1014–8. doi: 10.1093/jn/135.5.1014
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
131. Yokum S, Gearhardt AN, Harris JL, Brownell KD, Stice E. Individual differences in striatum activity to food commercials predict weight gain in adolescents. Obesity. (2014) 22:2544–51. doi: 10.1002/oby.20882
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
133. Chen S, Dong D, Jackson T, Zhuang Q, Chen H. Trait-based food-cravings are encoded by regional homogeneity in the parahippocampal gyrus. Appetite. (2017) 114:155–60. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.033
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
134. Nakata H, Sakamoto K, Ferretti A, Gianni Perrucci M, Del Gratta C, Kakigi R, et al. Somato-motor inhibitory processing in humans: an event-related functional MRI study. NeuroImage. (2008) 39:1858–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.041
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
135. Schmidt A, Müller F, Lenz C, Dolder PC, Schmid Y, Zanchi D, et al. Acute LSD effects on response inhibition neural networks. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:1464–73. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002914
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
136. Wodka EL, Mahone EM, Blankner JG, Larson JCG, Fotedar S, Denckla MB, et al. Evidence that response inhibition is a primary deficit in ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2007) 29:345–56. doi: 10.1080/13803390600678046
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
137. Spinelli S, Joel S, Nelson TE, Vasa RA, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Different neural patterns are associated with trials preceding inhibitory errors in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 50:705–15.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.03.014
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
138. Sheinkopf SJ, Lester BM, Sanes JN, Eliassen JC, Hutchison ER, Seifer R, et al. Functional MRI and response inhibition in children exposed to cocaine in utero. Dev Neurosci. (2009) 31:159–66. doi: 10.1159/000207503
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
139. Cohen RA. Cuneus. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer (2011). p. 756–7.
Google Scholar
140. Connolly CG, Bell RP, Foxe JJ, Garavan H. Dissociated grey matter changes with prolonged addiction and extended abstinence in cocaine users. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e59645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059645
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
141. Wang J, Fan Y, Dong Y, Ma M, Dong Y, Niu Y, et al. Combining gray matter volume in the cuneus and the cuneus-prefrontal connectivity may predict early relapse in abstinent alcohol-dependent patients. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0196860. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196860
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
142. Goldstein RZ, Tomasi D, Rajaram S, Cottone LA, Zhang L, Maloney T, et al. Role of the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex in processing drug cues in cocaine addiction. Neuroscience. (2007) 144:1153–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.024
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
143. Ng J, Stice E, Yokum S, Bohon C. An fMRI study of obesity, food reward, and perceived caloric density. Does a low-fat label make food less appealing? Appetite. (2011) 57:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.03.017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
144. Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Veldhuizen MG, Small DM. Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Abnorm Psychol. (2008) 117:924–35. doi: 10.1037/a0013600
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
145. Hill SY, Sharma V, Jones BL. Lifetime use of cannabis from longitudinal assessments, cannabinoid receptor (CNR1) variation, and reduced volume of the right anterior cingulate. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2016) 255:24–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.05.009
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
146. Hall W, Carter A, Forlini C. The brain disease model of addiction: is it supported by the evidence and has it delivered on its promises? Lancet Psychiatry. (2015) 2:105–10. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00126-6
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
147. Willeumier K, Taylor DV, Amen DG. Decreased cerebral blood flow in the limbic and prefrontal cortex using SPECT imaging in a cohort of completed suicides. Transl Psychiatry. (2011) 1:e28. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.28
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
148. Klonsky ED, May A. Rethinking impulsivity in suicide. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2010) 40:612–9. doi: 10.1521/suli.2010.40.6.612
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
149. Steele VR, Aharoni E, Munro GE, Calhoun VD, Nyalakanti P, Stevens MC, et al. A large scale (N = 102) functional neuroimaging study of response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Behav Brain Res. (2013) 256:529–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.06.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
150. Farr OM, Hu S, Zhang S, Li CR. Decreased saliency processing as a neural measure of Barratt impulsivity in healthy adults. NeuroImage. (2012) 63:1070–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.049
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
151. Garavan H, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (1999) 96:8301–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
152. Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Shared and selective neural correlates of inhibition, facilitation, and shifting processes during executive control. NeuroImage. (2010) 51:421–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.089
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
153. Gröpper S, Spengler S, Stuke H, Gawron CK, Parnack J, Gutwinski S, et al. Behavioral impulsivity mediates the relationship between decreased frontal gray matter volume and harmful alcohol drinking: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Psychiatr Res. (2016) 83:16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.006
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
154. Carmona S, Vilarroya O, Bielsa A, Trèmols V, Soliva JC, Rovira M, et al. Global and regional gray matter reductions in ADHD: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neurosci Lett. (2005) 389:88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.07.020
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
155. Schilling C, Kühn S, Romanowski A, Schubert F, Kathmann N, Gallinat J. Cortical thickness correlates with impulsiveness in healthy adults. NeuroImage. (2012) 59:824–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.058
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
156. Stoeckel LE, Murdaugh DL, Cox JE, Cook EW, Weller RE. Greater impulsivity is associated with decreased brain activation in obese women during a delay discounting task. Brain Imaging Behav. (2013) 7:116–28. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9201-4
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
157. Arias-Carrión O, Stamelou M, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Menéndez-González M, Pöppel E. Dopaminergic reward system: a short integrative review. Int Arch Med. (2010) 3:24. doi: 10.1186/1755-7682-3-24
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
162. Luijten M, Veltman DJ, van den Brink W, Hester R, Field M, Smits M, et al. Neurobiological substrate of smoking-related attentional bias. NeuroImage. (2011) 54:2374–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.064
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
166. Goll Y, Atlan G, Citri A. Attention: the claustrum. Trends Neurosci. (2015) 38:486–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.05.006
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
167. Wang G-J, Volkow ND, Logan J, Pappas NR, Wong CT, Zhu W, et al. Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet. (2001) 357:354–7. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03643-6
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
168. Josipovic Z. Nondual awareness: consciousness-as-such as non-representational reflexivity. In: Srinivasan N, editor. Progress in Brain Research. London: Elsevier (2019). p. 273–98.
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
169. van der Laan LN, de Ridder DTD, Viergever MA, Smeets PAM. The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage. (2011) 55:296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
170. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, 5th Edition: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
171. Hudson JI, Hiripi E, Pope HG, Kessler RC. The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 61:348–58. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.040
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
172. Wilfley DE, Friedman MA, Dounchis JZ, Stein RI, Welch RR, Ball SA. Comorbid psychopathology in binge eating disorder: relation to eating disorder severity at baseline and following treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2000) 68:641. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.641
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
173. Yanovski SZ, Nelson JE, Dubbert BK, Spitzer RL. Association of binge eating disorder and psychiatric comorbidity in obese subjects. Am J Psychiatry. (1993) 150:1472–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.10.1472
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
174. Johnson JG, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. Health problems, impairment and illnesses associated with bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder among primary care and obstetric gynaecology patients. Psychol Med. (2001) 31:1455–66. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004640
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
176. Mobbs O, Iglesias K, Golay A, Van der Linden M. Cognitive deficits in obese persons with and without binge eating disorder. Investigation using a mental flexibility task. Appetite. (2011) 57:263–71. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.023
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
177. Fernández-Aranda F, Pinheiro AP, Thornton LM, Berrettini WH, Crow S, Fichter MM, et al. Impulse control disorders in women with eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. (2008) 157:147–57. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.02.011
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
178. Bočková M, Chládek J, Jurák P, Halámek J, Rektor I. Executive functions processed in the frontal and lateral temporal cortices: intracerebral study. Clin Neurophysiol. (2007) 118:2625–36. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.07.025
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
179. Ojemann GA, Schoenfield-McNeill J. Activity of neurons in human temporal cortex during identification and memory for names and words. J Neurosci. (1999) 19:5674–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-13-05674.1999
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
180. Claus ED, Kiehl KA, Hutchison KE. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2011) 35:1209–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01455.x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
181. Goswami R, Dufort P, Tartaglia MC, Green RE, Crawley A, Tator CH, et al. Frontotemporal correlates of impulsivity and machine learning in retired professional athletes with a history of multiple concussions. Brain Struct Funct. (2016) 221:1911–25. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1012-0
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
182. Soloff P, Nutche J, Goradia D, Diwadkar V. Structural brain abnormalities in borderline personality disorder: a voxel-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2008) 164:223–36. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.02.003
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
183. Goethals I, Audenaert K, Jacobs F, den Eynde FV, Bernagie K, Kolindou A, et al. Brain perfusion SPECT in impulsivity-related personality disorders. Behav Brain Res. (2005) 157:187–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.06.022
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
184. King BM, Kass JM, Cadieux NL, Sam H, Neville KL, Arceneaux ER. Hyperphagia and obesity in female rats with temporal lobe lesions. Physiol Behav. (1993) 54:759–65. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90088-W
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
185. Kurth F, Levitt JG, Phillips OR, Luders E, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC. Relationships between gray matter, body mass index, and waist circumference in healthy adults. Hum Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:1737–46. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22021
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
186. Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K, Inoue K, Goto R, Okada K, et al. Relationship between body mass index and gray matter volume in 1,428 healthy individuals. Obesity. (2008) 16:119–24. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.4
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
187. Raji CA, Ho AJ, Parikshak NN, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, et al. Brain structure and obesity. Hum Brain Mapp. (2010) 31:353–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20870
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
189. Zhang Y, Ji G, Xu M, Cai W, Zhu Q, Qian L, et al. Recovery of brain structural abnormalities in morbidly obese patients after bariatric surgery. Int J Obes. (2016) 40:1558–65. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2016.98
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
190. Gustafson D, Lissner L, Bengtsson C, Björkelund C, Skoog I. A 24-year follow-up of body mass index and cerebral atrophy. Neurology. (2004) 63:1876–81. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000141850.47773.5F
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
191. Lowe MR, van Steenburgh J, Ochner C, Coletta M. Neural correlates of individual differences related to appetite. Physiol Behav. (2009) 97:561–71. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.04.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
193. Fuentes P, Barrós-Loscertales A, Bustamante JC, Rosell P, Costumero V, Ávila C. Individual differences in the behavioral inhibition system are associated with orbitofrontal cortex and precuneus gray matter volume. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2012) 12:491–8. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0099-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
194. Ikeda A, Ohara S, Matsumoto R, Kunieda T, Nagamine T, Miyamoto S, et al. Role of primary sensorimotor cortices in generating inhibitory motor response in humans. Brain J Neurol. (2000) 123:1710–21. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.8.1710
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar